Stay up to date with notifications from The Independent

Notifications can be managed in browser preferences.

Does practice make perfect? Not nearly as much as you might think

 

Elisa Criado
Thursday 03 July 2014 08:13 BST
Comments
Practice accounts for around 21 per cent of individual differences in music, and for less than one per cent of differences in skill level in professions.
Practice accounts for around 21 per cent of individual differences in music, and for less than one per cent of differences in skill level in professions. (Corbis)

For years we have been told that the more time we spend knuckling down practising our violin scales, golf swings and chess moves, the more we grow to master them. But have you ever wondered why despite all those hours of dedication, so few people actually get really good at their chosen skill? It’s a question that has kept psychologists busy for decades, and according to a new study published in Psychological Science, the answer is that practice matters far less than we think.

A research team led by Brooke Macnamara of Princeton University analysed the data of 88 scientific studies covering the skill areas of music, games, sports, education, and professions. They found that although there is a positive relationship between practice and mastery, practice only accounts for a small amount of the variation in skill level; about 12 per cent across all of the areas of expertise.

According to Macnamara, "Deliberate practice is unquestionably important, but not nearly as important as proponents of the view have claimed."

The relationship between practice and skill level was found to be different for each of the domains studied. Exercises to improve performance seem to have the greatest effect when it comes to games; with 26 per cent of individual differences in this domain being attributed to prior training. However, less than one per cent of the diversity in skilfulness at work appears to be down to deliberate attempts to increase competence. Withing the fields of music and education, these numbers are 21 per cent and four per cent, respectively. The aggregated data also show that the more predictable an activity is, the more worthwhile it is to invest in honing the relevant skills. Running ability, for example, was more closely linked to the amount of practice put in than the capability to handle an aviation emergency.

The scientific debate over whether experts are ‘born’ or ‘made’ has been around since the advent of genetics. Twenty years ago an extremely influential piece of research by K. Anders Ericsson and colleagues posited that many characteristics thought to reflect innate talent were actually the result of intense practice over an extended period of time. This research and the ideas it generated have been discussed in many popular books such as Levitt and Dubner’s SuperFreakonomics, and fit well within the popular view that hard work will get anyone where they want to be. According to Macnamara and her team, this interpretation is not supported by the empirical evidence.

Having demonstrated that practice on its own does not make perfect, Macnamara says: “For scientists, the important question now is, what else matters?”

The researchers suggest that one of the factors that may play a role in achieving expertise is the age at which a person starts practising, as there may be an optimal developmental period for acquiring complex skills. However, the lion’s share of the variation in competence could be down to a mix of general intelligence and specific abilities such as working memory, which affect an individual’s performance across a wide range of pursuits.

Join our commenting forum

Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies

Comments

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in