Stay up to date with notifications from The Independent

Notifications can be managed in browser preferences.

The Independent's journalism is supported by our readers. When you purchase through links on our site, we may earn commission.

Enough to make your skin crawl: The evolutionary importance of feeling disgust

The urge to gag or vomit when we swallow something we expect will do us harm is fairly obvious but why might something like a tick make you feel sick too? Sabrina Imbler writes

Thursday 12 August 2021 00:01 BST
Comments
Faintly disgusting: Researchers saw extreme physiological reactions to vile videos
Faintly disgusting: Researchers saw extreme physiological reactions to vile videos (Getty)

Nausea is our trusty personal bodyguard, it appears.

Feeling nauseated is widely accepted to be an evolutionary defence that protects people from pathogens and parasites. The urge to gag or vomit is “well-suited” to defend ourselves against things we swallow that might contain pathogens, according to Tom Kupfer, a psychological scientist at Nottingham Trent University. But vomiting is somewhat futile against a tick, an ectoparasite that latches on to skin, not stomachs.

In an experiment that produced both stomach-churning and skin-crawling sensations – I can confirm these and some other physiological responses first-hand.

Kupfer and Daniel Fessler, an evolutionary anthropologist from the University of California, Los Angeles, argue in a paper published in the journal Proceedings of the Royal Society B that humans have evolved to defend themselves against ectoparasites through a skin response that elicits scratching.

Although some experts say more research is needed, the findings align with some understandings of the evolution of disgust.

Cécile Sarabian, a cognitive ecologist studying animal disgust at the Kyoto University Primate Research Institute in Japan, says: “It makes sense to have developed adaptive defensive strategies against the ‘nasty’ ones.”

The disgusting investigation began in 2017 at Chicheley Hall, Buckinghamshire. Kupfer was presenting findings to colleagues on trypophobia, an aversion to clustered holes. His data showed that participants with trypophobia often reacted to holey images with the urge to itch or scratch, sometimes to the point of bleeding. Kupfer suggested that trypophobia might not represent fear but rather a disgust reaction to signs of parasites or infectious diseases, which can both result in clusters of lesions or pustules.

Kupfer’s presentation included images such as lotus seed pods or foam bubbles. At one point during the presentation, a distressed researcher began shouting for Kupfer to take an image down.

When one hole closes, another opens. Fessler approached Kupfer after the presentation and the two researchers began talking about how the human body might have two types of defensive responses in reaction to certain threats. If nausea and vomiting protect against ingesting dangerous microbes, scratching might protect against ectoparasites. They then began working on a review paper that was published in 2018.

For the new paper, Kupfer and Fessler developed a study where they showed people a series of 90-second videos – a suggestive medley of pathogens and ectoparasites – and asked the participants about their emotional and physical response.

Selecting the videos was an art. Kupfer says: “We didn’t want people just to say, ‘It’s disgusting’. We wanted the physiological sensations that accompany the response: nausea, gagging, itching and scratching.”

So Kupfer pored through YouTube with Sonia Alas and Tiffany Hwang, then undergraduate students at UCLA. They watched and debated for hours to select the most vile footage. Many options were too weak, such as footage of “mildly mouldy food,” Kupfer says. “We wanted faeces, some sort of infection.”

Kupfer’s dream came true. The final ectoparasite clips included a kitten riddled with fleas, a nightmarish bedbug infestation and a beauty shot of a mosquito sucking blood. The final pathogen clips included meat pulsing with maggots, an infected arm lesion oozing pus – Fessler called it the “pus volcano” – and a clump of earwax as dark as an asteroid.

The meat was Kupfer’s own creation; unable to find an adequately disgusting video of rotten food, he left a slab of meat in his garden for two weeks and returned when it “seemed maximally disgusting”.

The video that the researchers found most disgusting – Dirty festival toilets – has since been removed from YouTube. I tried to watch every video used in the experiment. I did not vomit but I did experience heart palpitations and had to sit in my bathroom with the lights off for several minutes until I stopped seeing the pus volcano. Missing out on the festival toilets, it seemed, was an act of self-care.

If you understand why you respond to the world in the ways that you do, then you have agency

The researchers conducted essentially the same experiment three times, twice in the United States and once in China, surveying more than 1,000 people. In all three surveys, participants had distinct reactions to the ectoparasite videos when compared with the pathogen videos. When watching ectoparasites, participants reported more urges to itch and scratch, theoretically protecting the surface of their skin from danger. And when watching pathogens, the participants reported more feelings of nausea and urges to vomit.

The researchers plan to expand this project internationally to see how ectoparasite disgust responses vary in different countries and in different languages. Understanding the nuances of disgust, they say, could inform our understanding of disorders such as delusional parasitosis, the mistaken belief that parasites have invaded the body.

Bunmi O Olatunji, a psychologist at Vanderbilt University, Tennessee, who was not involved in the research, says that he considers the new paper’s results too preliminary to make inferences about clinical conditions but it does offer, “interesting possibilities for thinking about the mechanism by which disgust may contribute to the development and maintenance of skin-picking disorder”.

Fessler says: “Your mind is a compilation of a whole bunch of mechanisms produced by natural selection. If you understand why you respond to the world in the ways that you do, then you have agency.”

After we hung up, I noticed I had been scratching a bite on my leg that I did not know about beforehand. At least, I think it was.

This article originally appeared in The New York Times.

Join our commenting forum

Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies

Comments

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in